0 0,00*
Studying Social Networks
Marina Hennig, Ulrik Brandes, Jürgen Pfeffer, Ines Mergel

Studying Social Networks

A Guide to Empirical Research

Vorübergehend nicht lieferbar, voraussichtlich ab 2024 lieferbar


Produktdetails

Verlag
Campus
Erschienen
2012
Sprache
English
Seiten
218
Infos
218 Seiten
49 Abbildungen
22.8 cm x 15.2 cm
ISBN
978-3-593-39763-4

Inhaltsverzeichnis

Preface . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9

How to Use this Book . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13

1.1 The Construction of Social Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
1.2 Social Network Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
1.2.1 The Community Question . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
1.2.2 Viral Marketing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20
1.2.3 Corporate Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21
1.3 Exercises . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25

2. Research Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
2.1 Social Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29
2.2 Networks as Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
2.2.1 Explanatory Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
2.2.2 Dependent Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37
2.3 Typology of Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
2.3.1 Complete Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49
2.3.2 Ego-centered Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52
2.4 Longitudinal Network Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55
2.5 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57
2.6 Exercises . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59



3. Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61
3.1 Kinds of Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62
3.1.1 Units and Levels . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62
3.1.2 Organization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67
3.1.3 Which Data for Which Type of Network? . . . . . 72
3.2 Data Collection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
3.2.1 Sources . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
3.2.2 Boundary Specification . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83
3.2.3 Alter Recall . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
3.3 Quality Issues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
3.4 Ethical Considerations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97
3.5 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99
3.6 Exercises . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100

4. Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
4.1 Mathematical Representation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104
4.1.1 Graphs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106
4.1.2 Ego-Centered Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109
4.1.3 Two-Mode Networks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
4.2 Indexing and Grouping . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111
4.2.1 Dyads as the Unit of Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112
4.2.2 Network Characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
4.2.3 Centrality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123
4.2.4 Cohesion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130
4.2.5 Roles . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134
4.2.6 Blockmodeling . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137
4.3 Modeling . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140
4.3.1 Idealized Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
4.3.2 Exponential-Family Random Graph Models . . . . 143
4.4 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145
4.5 Exercises . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146

5. Visualization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 149
5.1 Graphical Representation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
5.1.1 Sociogram . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
5.1.2 Sociomatrix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155
5.1.3 Two-Mode Network Representations . . . . . . . . 159
5.2 Multi variate Information Visualization . . . . . . . . . . . . 162
5.2.1 Substance-Based Layout . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165
5.2.2 Other Graphical Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169
5.3 Information Layering . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169
5.3.1 Filtering . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 170
5.3.2 Level of Detail . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
5.3.3 Micro/Macro Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175
5.4 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177
5.5 Exercises . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179

6. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183

List of Figures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 189

List of Boxes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 191

About the Authors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 193

Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 195

Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 211

Textauszug

Relations matter. You knew this, of course - Why else would you be in- terested in learning about social network analysis? The real questions are: How, where, when, and why do they matter? And, more pragmatically, how can you show that they do?

This book is organized along the process of an empirical study of so- cial networks. It thus provides a guideline and orientation. While we concentrate on the things that are not treated in textbooks on empirical studies of population samples (i.e., non-relational studies), we still think that the book is largely self-contained.
So, what is the subject of a network study?

1.1 The Construction of Social Networks

It has become commonplace to refer to interacting or otherwise depen- dent entities as networks. The phenomena described as networks range from the social interactions of human beings and the flow of goods be- tween countries to gene regulation and railroad infrastructures. What do these examples have in common that leads us to think we can model and analyze them in similar ways?

Some of the phenomena referred to as networks are real in the sense that their existence does not depend on our perspective. Online social networking services, for example, are technology-enabled products. As such they have well-defined elements. A friending protocol specifies the sequences of actions that yield a link between two user accounts. The immanent meaning of such a link is unambiguous. We may refer to the web of linked accounts as a network or not, in any case, it is represented in the service provider's databases.

However, the social network of human beings who own accounts in the above system is an inferred, construed object. It has no independent existence and is thus always subject to interpretation. In these cases, the use of the term network is that of a model or metaphor; it does not denote an unambiguous object but a perspective.

As a metaphor the term "network" is very graphic, immediately evoking images of points and connecting line segments. Metaphors are very useful for memorization and creative thinking. However, it is not neces- sarily obvious which aspects of a metaphor correspond to actual proper- ties of that which is represented, and which aspects do not.

Another pitfall of metaphors and models alike is the use of similar rep- resentations for weakly related phenomena. By abstracting from the non- essential (with respect to a specific perspective), otherwise invalid com- monalities and conclusions may emerge. To illustrate this point, consider (statistical) "distributions" as another example of a representation. If both the distribution of life-expectancy in the east of Austria and the household income in a suburb of Berlin are unimodal (i.e., have a single peak), does this imply that there is a relation between these two phenomena? We as- sume that you would not think so, but it appears to be much more tempt- ing to speculate about such relations when two networks exhibit similar features because it is more easily forgotten that they are simplifying and homogenizing, reductionist representations.

The study of social networks is, hence, the study of a particular type of representation in social science contexts (Freeman 1989). Therefore, social networks are constructs and do not exist as such. They are repre- sentations, in which aspects of a social phenomenon - aspects that seem to be relevant in a specific context and for a specific purpose - are expressed in ways more amenable to scientific scrutiny.

Since there are no social networks per se, it is a linguistic simplification when we say that we are studying social networks. In fact we are studying social phenomena by means of network representations. This is carried It appears that the term "social network" was coined in Barnes (1954), in which pre- cisely this image is evoked out by gathering data about aspects of a phenomenon and organizing the data in a convenient form, by applying methods that produce additional, derived data, and translating these back to the realm of the phenomenon. Clearly, this is no different from other empirical investigations. What is distinct in network analysis, however, are the kinds of data and methods, and the reasoning that motivates network representations and justifies the interpretation of results.

Hauptbeschreibung

Das Interesse an der Netzwerkanalyse nimmt rapide zu. Bisher fehlt es jedoch an empirisch orientierten Einführungen. Das interdisziplinäre Autorenteam führt daher praxisorientiert in die Grundlagen und Methoden der empirischen Analyse sozialer Netzwerke ein. Schritt für Schritt wird der Forschungsprozess von der Untersuchungsplanung über die Auswertungsmethodik bis zur Präsentation der Ergebnisse erläutert. Damit ist das Lehrbuch für den Einsatz in Lehre, Forschung und Praxis geeignet.

This textbook provides an introduction to the process of empirical network research. In an action-oriented approach, it features explicated learning goals, numerous reference examples, and exercises that facilitate successful learning. Integrating their different disciplinary perspectives, the authors address an interdisciplinary audience of teachers, researchers, and practitioners alike.

Über den AutorIn

Marina Hennig ist Professorin für Netzwerkforschung und Familiensoziologie an der Universität Mainz. Ulrik Brandes ist Professor für Algorithmik am Fachbereich Informatik der Universität Konstanz. Jürgen Pfeffer ist PostDoc am Institute for Software Research der Carnegie Mellon University, Pennsylvania. Ines Mergel ist Assistant Professor für Public Administration an der Syracuse University, New York.